Nike Air Cleats Huarache 2k Filth Elite Pink PE Baseball Cleats Air Mens Sz 16 455b49

Nike Air Cleats Huarache 2k Filth Elite Pink PE Baseball Cleats Air Mens Sz 16 455b49
No matter big shoes, children's shoes, sports shoes, casual shoes, pay attention to functionality, and wear comfortable beauty, play the functions of elasticity, lightness, breathability, and no fatigue, which meets the requirements of modern people for shoes. Support and breathable enough for baby .Modern people's hard work, it is best to add a pair of active shoes, wear the right ergonomic shoes, to be considerate of the pretty jade feet, make the feet comfortable and antibacterial, easy to walk, walk the road to be comfortable, Looks like alive.
This is a cropped closeup of a senior couple enjoying a game of chess

Nike Air Cleats Huarache 2k Filth Elite Pink PE Baseball Cleats Air Mens Sz 16 455b49

Women’s cognitive functioning past middle age may be affected by the degree of gender equality in the country they live in, according to new findings from Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

“This research is a first attempt to shed light on important, but understudied, adverse consequences of gender inequality on women’s health in later life,” explains researcher Eric Bonsang of University Paris-Dauphine and Columbia University, lead author on the study. “It shows that women living in gender-equal countries have better cognitive test scores later in life than women living in gender-unequal societies. Moreover, in countries that became more gender-equal over time, women’s cognitive performance improved relative to men’s.”

Bonsang and colleagues Vegard Skirbekk (Norwegian Institute of Public Health and Columbia University) and Ursula Staudinger (Columbia University) had noticed that the differences in men’s and women’s scores on cognitive tests varied widely across countries. In countries in Northern Europe, for example, women tend to outperform men on memory tests, while the opposite seems to be true in several Southern European countries.

“This observation triggered our curiosity to try to understand what could cause such variations across countries,” says Bonsang.

While economic and socioeconomic factors likely play an important role, Bonsang, Skirbekk, and Staudinger wondered whether sociocultural factors such as attitudes about gender roles might also contribute to the variation in gender differences in cognitive performance around the globe. They hypothesized that women who live in a society with more traditional attitudes about gender roles would likely have less access to opportunities for education and employment and would, therefore, show lower cognitive performance later in life compared with men of the same age.

The researchers analyzed cognitive performance data for participants between the ages of 50 and 93, drawn from multiple nationally representative surveys including the US Health and Retirement Study; the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe; the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing; and the World Health Organization Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health. Together, the surveys provided data for a total of 27 countries.

All of the surveys include an episodic memory task to measure cognitive performance. Participants heard a list of 10 words and were asked to recall as many as they could immediately; in some of the surveys, participants again recalled as many words as they could after a delay. Additionally, some of the surveys included a task intended to assess executive function in which participants named as many animals as they could within 1 minute.

To gauge gender-role attitudes, the researchers focused on participants’ self-reported agreement with the statement, “When jobs are scarce, men should have more right to a job than women.”

Overall, the data showed considerable variability in gender differences in cognitive performance across countries. In some countries, women outperformed men—the female advantage in cognitive performance was highest in Sweden. In other countries, however, men outperformed women—the male advantage was highest in Ghana.

As the researchers hypothesized, increasingly traditional gender-role attitudes were linked with decreasing cognitive performance among women across countries. In other words, women in countries with less traditional attitudes were likely to have better cognitive performance later in life relative to women in more traditional countries.

Bonsang and colleagues noted that changes in gender-role attitudes within a country over time were associated with changes in women’s cognitive performance relative to men.

Although the data are correlational in nature, several more detailed analyses point toward a causal relationship. These analyses suggest that gender-role attitudes may play a notable role in important outcomes for women across different countries, the researchers argue.

“These findings reinforce the need for policies aiming at reducing gender inequalities as we show that consequences go beyond the labor market and income inequalities,” says Bonsang. “It also shows how important it is to consider seemingly intangible influences, such as cultural attitudes and values, when trying to understand cognitive aging.”

“In future work, we plan to disentangle the effect of gender-role attitudes on gender difference in cognition—via the impacts of those attitudes on institutions, politics and labor market characteristics—from the impact of beliefs of women associated with gender-role attitudes,” Bonsang says.

We use data from Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) Release 2.6.0, English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) Release 19, the RAND Corporation Health and Retirement Study (HRS) Version N, and Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health (SAGE) Wave 1. The SHARE project has been primarily funded by the European Commission (see http://www.share-project.org for the full list of funders). The ELSA project has been funded by a consortium of U.K. government departments and the U.S. National Institute on Aging (see http://www.elsa-project.ac.uk/funders for the full list of funders). The HRS project is funded by the Social Security Administration and the U.S. National Institute on Aging


Published July 31, 2017
NIKE Air Flight Huarache Sneakers SHOES US_10.5 / EUR_44.5Nike Air Python Lux SP sz 8,Nike Downshifter 8 Men's Running Shoes Size 13,Nike Zoom Superfly R4 Spikes Running Shoes Black Green Silver 526626-035 SZ 12,NIke SB Check Solar Men’s Sz 11 Skateboarding Shoes Industrial Blue 843895-410,Nike Lupinek Flyknit Cargo Khaki ACG Men Sneakerboots Outdoors Shoes 862505-300,NIKE ROSHE One PREM CB MEN'S Running SHOE 807609 003 Blk/Green Size 9.5Nike Air Flight Falcon Red White Black Grey Shoes 397204-023 Sz 11,Nike Zoom Rev Shoes size 11.5 gray and whiteNIKE BLAZER MID TOP PRM GUM SOLES 429988-099 SB Men's Off White Suede Size 11.5,Nike KD V All Star Area 72 ASG Galaxy Men 8,NIKE AIR MAX TORCH 4 MEN'S SHOES obsidian white 343846-400 Sz 10,NIKE SPECIAL FIELD AIR FORCE 1 MID 917753 005,NEW NIKE VAPOR SPEED TURF FOOTBALL LACROSSE CLEATS BLACK WHITE 833408-017 SZ 8Nike Air Max AM 90 Essential Black Silver 537384-047 Size 12.5 Men's EUCNike 749884 005 NIKE ZOOM KOBE VENOMENON 5 Size 13 Black Basketball Shoes,Nike SB Eric Koston 2 Max Skate shoes trainers sneakers 631047 434 NEW+BOX,Nike Air Zoom Ultra Men's Tennis Shoes Volt Black Multiple Sizes NEW 845007 701,Mens Nike Air Max Light,Nike Zoom Rival S 8 Track & Field Men's Shoes Size 9.5,AIR JORDAN FLIGHT REMIX SIZE 10 BRAND NEW,Nike Air Griffey Max 2 Gs Black Yellow Size 6y,Nike Archive '83.M Loyal Star Blue/Grey/Royal Limited 747245-402 Size 11 New Men,Nike Blazer Mid PRM Casual Mens Size 9.5 Shoes Game Red Leather 429988-604,Nike SB Dunk Mid “Beavis And Butthead” Size 13,VNDS Air Trainer I Mid Premium SP Fragment "Rust/White" sz 9.5Nike KD VI Night Vision Size 9.5 (Offer),NIKE AIR JORDAN 8 VIII RETRO PLAYOFF 2009 BLACK OG 305381 061 RED SIZE 9,nike kd 6 size 13,Nike men's air pegasus A/T Sz 11 Mars Stone/Black (924469 601) Retail $ 120 NIB