DS NIKE 2007 VANDAL VINTAGE 9.5 OG RETRO PINE GREEN 9.5 VINTAGE DUNK TERMINATOR FORCE AIR 1 d5fa82

DS NIKE 2007 VANDAL VINTAGE 9.5 OG RETRO PINE GREEN 9.5 VINTAGE DUNK TERMINATOR FORCE AIR 1 d5fa82

Item specifics

Condition:
New with box: A brand-new, unused, and unworn item (including handmade items) in the original packaging (such as ... Read moreabout the condition
Brand: Nike/ Jordan
Style: Fashion Sneakers US Shoe Size (Men's): 9.5
UPC: Does not apply
This is a cropped closeup of a senior couple enjoying a game of chess

DS NIKE 2007 VANDAL VINTAGE 9.5 OG RETRO PINE GREEN 9.5 VINTAGE DUNK TERMINATOR FORCE AIR 1 d5fa82

Women’s cognitive functioning past middle age may be affected by the degree of gender equality in the country they live in, according to new findings from Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

“This research is a first attempt to shed light on important, but understudied, adverse consequences of gender inequality on women’s health in later life,” explains researcher Eric Bonsang of University Paris-Dauphine and Columbia University, lead author on the study. “It shows that women living in gender-equal countries have better cognitive test scores later in life than women living in gender-unequal societies. Moreover, in countries that became more gender-equal over time, women’s cognitive performance improved relative to men’s.”

Bonsang and colleagues Vegard Skirbekk (Norwegian Institute of Public Health and Columbia University) and Ursula Staudinger (Columbia University) had noticed that the differences in men’s and women’s scores on cognitive tests varied widely across countries. In countries in Northern Europe, for example, women tend to outperform men on memory tests, while the opposite seems to be true in several Southern European countries.

“This observation triggered our curiosity to try to understand what could cause such variations across countries,” says Bonsang.

While economic and socioeconomic factors likely play an important role, Bonsang, Skirbekk, and Staudinger wondered whether sociocultural factors such as attitudes about gender roles might also contribute to the variation in gender differences in cognitive performance around the globe. They hypothesized that women who live in a society with more traditional attitudes about gender roles would likely have less access to opportunities for education and employment and would, therefore, show lower cognitive performance later in life compared with men of the same age.

The researchers analyzed cognitive performance data for participants between the ages of 50 and 93, drawn from multiple nationally representative surveys including the US Health and Retirement Study; the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe; the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing; and the World Health Organization Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health. Together, the surveys provided data for a total of 27 countries.

All of the surveys include an episodic memory task to measure cognitive performance. Participants heard a list of 10 words and were asked to recall as many as they could immediately; in some of the surveys, participants again recalled as many words as they could after a delay. Additionally, some of the surveys included a task intended to assess executive function in which participants named as many animals as they could within 1 minute.

To gauge gender-role attitudes, the researchers focused on participants’ self-reported agreement with the statement, “When jobs are scarce, men should have more right to a job than women.”

Overall, the data showed considerable variability in gender differences in cognitive performance across countries. In some countries, women outperformed men—the female advantage in cognitive performance was highest in Sweden. In other countries, however, men outperformed women—the male advantage was highest in Ghana.

As the researchers hypothesized, increasingly traditional gender-role attitudes were linked with decreasing cognitive performance among women across countries. In other words, women in countries with less traditional attitudes were likely to have better cognitive performance later in life relative to women in more traditional countries.

Bonsang and colleagues noted that changes in gender-role attitudes within a country over time were associated with changes in women’s cognitive performance relative to men.

Although the data are correlational in nature, several more detailed analyses point toward a causal relationship. These analyses suggest that gender-role attitudes may play a notable role in important outcomes for women across different countries, the researchers argue.

“These findings reinforce the need for policies aiming at reducing gender inequalities as we show that consequences go beyond the labor market and income inequalities,” says Bonsang. “It also shows how important it is to consider seemingly intangible influences, such as cultural attitudes and values, when trying to understand cognitive aging.”

“In future work, we plan to disentangle the effect of gender-role attitudes on gender difference in cognition—via the impacts of those attitudes on institutions, politics and labor market characteristics—from the impact of beliefs of women associated with gender-role attitudes,” Bonsang says.

We use data from Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) Release 2.6.0, English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) Release 19, the RAND Corporation Health and Retirement Study (HRS) Version N, and Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health (SAGE) Wave 1. The SHARE project has been primarily funded by the European Commission (see http://www.share-project.org for the full list of funders). The ELSA project has been funded by a consortium of U.K. government departments and the U.S. National Institute on Aging (see http://www.elsa-project.ac.uk/funders for the full list of funders). The HRS project is funded by the Social Security Administration and the U.S. National Institute on Aging


Published July 31, 2017
ECCO Men's Biarritz Modern Brogue Oxford - Choose SZ/Color,Antonio Maurizi Penny Loafers - Men's Size 8, Bordeauxadidas Men's Shoes 203778 Grey 28.5cm,Sperry Top-Sider Billfish Slip On - Choose SZ/Color,Men's MEPHISTO 'Vito' Navy Blue Nubuck Perforated Sneakers Size US 12,Wolverine 6" Merlin Peak AG Waterproof Composite Toe Boot Sz 9EW Black NIB $140ADIDAS CHAUSSURE GAZELLE - NVYWHT - 6.5 (4056566314941),Men/Women Andrew Charles Mens Sneaker White ED feature Good market Lightweight shoes,ASTORFLEX Greenflex E Nero men's shoes boots US 11 (pv:),Polo Ralph Lauren Tynedale Mixed Media Boots Black Leather Chambray Canvas $109,Men's Alessandro Dell'acqua Sneakers Size 45 Euro 11 US,Hugo Boss Shoes Maze_Lowp_knit Sneakers Men Blue Brand New,Merrell Men's Jungle Moc Waterproof Slip-On Shoe,Gunsmoke,10 M USRockport Let's Walk Men's Slip-on Comfort Shoe - All Colors - All Sizes,Polo Ralph Lauren Men's Ranger Boots Port Nubuck 812570443-001,men's shoes UMBERTO LUCIANI 7,5 () sneakers brown AP354-B,Kenneth Cole New York Men's Got a Clue Slip-on Loafer - Choose SZ/Color,Kenneth Cole New York Men's Text Me Oxford,Hush Puppies Nathan Theron Mens Oxford- Choose SZ/Color.,Lucky Press, LLC Lucien Piccard Mens LP- Eiger Black Leather Watch,Lacoste Shoes Turnier 316 1 CAM Men's Leather Suede Casual Fashion Sneakers GreyMen's/Women's Warson Brands- Pick SZ/Color. High-quality the most economical Preferential price,Alexander Mcqueen by Puma BRACE leather 10.5 nwb,NEW Converse CTAS Pro Hi Sean Pablo White Skate Mens Womens Unisex Shoes z1,Puma Men's Match Emboss Lthr Fashion Sneaker - Choose SZ/Color,Kenneth Cole New York Men's Say Hello Oxford,DIADORA MEN'S SHOES SUEDE TRAINERS SNEAKERS NEW CAMARO BLUE 7A8,Frye Phillip Lug Wingtip Chukka Men's 8.0 Cognac Retail $398,New Fashion Mens Embroidery Rhinestone Snake Velvet Mules Loafers Retro SlippersHARUTA 906 WIDE 4E DARK BROWN CALF LEATHER PENNY LOAFERS - / / US 7.5,