New Shoe, Balance Womens WW411WT2 Walking Shoe, New White, 8 2A US 22800e

New Shoe, Balance Womens WW411WT2 Walking Shoe, New White, 8 2A US 22800e

Item specifics

Condition:
New with box: A brand-new, unused, and unworn item (including handmade items) in the original packaging (such as ... Read moreabout the condition
UPC: 0889516077030
Size: 8 2A(N) US Brand: New Balance
Style: sport Model: WW411V2 Walking Shoe-W
US Shoe Size (Women's): 8 MPN: WW411V2 Walking Shoe-W
Size Type: Regular Color: White
Material Type: Leather EAN: 0889516077030
This is a cropped closeup of a senior couple enjoying a game of chess

New Shoe, Balance Womens WW411WT2 Walking Shoe, New White, 8 2A US 22800e

Women’s cognitive functioning past middle age may be affected by the degree of gender equality in the country they live in, according to new findings from Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

“This research is a first attempt to shed light on important, but understudied, adverse consequences of gender inequality on women’s health in later life,” explains researcher Eric Bonsang of University Paris-Dauphine and Columbia University, lead author on the study. “It shows that women living in gender-equal countries have better cognitive test scores later in life than women living in gender-unequal societies. Moreover, in countries that became more gender-equal over time, women’s cognitive performance improved relative to men’s.”

Bonsang and colleagues Vegard Skirbekk (Norwegian Institute of Public Health and Columbia University) and Ursula Staudinger (Columbia University) had noticed that the differences in men’s and women’s scores on cognitive tests varied widely across countries. In countries in Northern Europe, for example, women tend to outperform men on memory tests, while the opposite seems to be true in several Southern European countries.

“This observation triggered our curiosity to try to understand what could cause such variations across countries,” says Bonsang.

While economic and socioeconomic factors likely play an important role, Bonsang, Skirbekk, and Staudinger wondered whether sociocultural factors such as attitudes about gender roles might also contribute to the variation in gender differences in cognitive performance around the globe. They hypothesized that women who live in a society with more traditional attitudes about gender roles would likely have less access to opportunities for education and employment and would, therefore, show lower cognitive performance later in life compared with men of the same age.

The researchers analyzed cognitive performance data for participants between the ages of 50 and 93, drawn from multiple nationally representative surveys including the US Health and Retirement Study; the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe; the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing; and the World Health Organization Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health. Together, the surveys provided data for a total of 27 countries.

All of the surveys include an episodic memory task to measure cognitive performance. Participants heard a list of 10 words and were asked to recall as many as they could immediately; in some of the surveys, participants again recalled as many words as they could after a delay. Additionally, some of the surveys included a task intended to assess executive function in which participants named as many animals as they could within 1 minute.

To gauge gender-role attitudes, the researchers focused on participants’ self-reported agreement with the statement, “When jobs are scarce, men should have more right to a job than women.”

Overall, the data showed considerable variability in gender differences in cognitive performance across countries. In some countries, women outperformed men—the female advantage in cognitive performance was highest in Sweden. In other countries, however, men outperformed women—the male advantage was highest in Ghana.

As the researchers hypothesized, increasingly traditional gender-role attitudes were linked with decreasing cognitive performance among women across countries. In other words, women in countries with less traditional attitudes were likely to have better cognitive performance later in life relative to women in more traditional countries.

Bonsang and colleagues noted that changes in gender-role attitudes within a country over time were associated with changes in women’s cognitive performance relative to men.

Although the data are correlational in nature, several more detailed analyses point toward a causal relationship. These analyses suggest that gender-role attitudes may play a notable role in important outcomes for women across different countries, the researchers argue.

“These findings reinforce the need for policies aiming at reducing gender inequalities as we show that consequences go beyond the labor market and income inequalities,” says Bonsang. “It also shows how important it is to consider seemingly intangible influences, such as cultural attitudes and values, when trying to understand cognitive aging.”

“In future work, we plan to disentangle the effect of gender-role attitudes on gender difference in cognition—via the impacts of those attitudes on institutions, politics and labor market characteristics—from the impact of beliefs of women associated with gender-role attitudes,” Bonsang says.

We use data from Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) Release 2.6.0, English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) Release 19, the RAND Corporation Health and Retirement Study (HRS) Version N, and Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health (SAGE) Wave 1. The SHARE project has been primarily funded by the European Commission (see http://www.share-project.org for the full list of funders). The ELSA project has been funded by a consortium of U.K. government departments and the U.S. National Institute on Aging (see http://www.elsa-project.ac.uk/funders for the full list of funders). The HRS project is funded by the Social Security Administration and the U.S. National Institute on Aging


Published July 31, 2017
Nike Free 5.0+ Women's Running Shoe 580591 061 Size 11.5,ADIDAS ORIGINALS TORSION ZX FLUX NPS 43-48.5 NEW equipment 500 700 750 850,Adidas BY1970 Women Stella McCartney Pure boost X shoes blue green Sneakers,NEW Womens Sz 8.5 NIKE Air Force 1 Prem 07 315186 221 Sneakers ShoesConverse All Star Iridescent Shroud 553019C Black Egret Women Size US 5 6,Nike Blazer Low LE Women Lifestyle Shoes AV9370-001,Nike Air Max 1 GS Kids Womens Running Shoes Wolf Grey White Blue SizeWOMENS ADIDAS CRAZY ADV SZ 9.5 METALLIC CORE BLACK WHITE CQ2842Nike Dualtone Racer Elemental Rose Light Carbon Pink Running Shoes Women Sz 7.5,ASICS Gel-DS Trainer 20 Blue/Yellow/Pink Running Shoes Women's US Size 5.5 (I,0),STEVEN by Steve Madden Women's Nyssa Sneaker,Keds Women's Cali LL Studio Jersey Sneaker - Choose SZ/Color,New Balance WS247UE B Grey White Women Running Casual Shoes Sneakers WS247UEB,W Air Huarache Run Ultra Pink NikeKeds Women's Ace Leather Sneaker White/Blush (Leather) Sneakers,Women Adidas CP9779 Energy Cloud V Running shoes grey Sneakers,NEW ECCO WOMENS CRUISE SPORT ANKLE STRAP SANDAL,$420 PRADA SPORT LOW TOP LEATHER CAP TOE SNEAKER 36.5,New Nike Womens Zoom Vapor 9.5 Tour Tennis Shoes 631475-164 Sz 11,Womens NIKE AIR MAX MOTION LW SE Armory Blue Trainers 844895 403,Nike Air Max 95 Vast Grey Habanero Red Used US 9 WGentlemen/Ladies New Balance Women's Ws574 fashion online shop best seller,Saucony Womens Sapphire Running Shoe- Pick SZ/Color.,ROSHE TWO BR WOMAN SIZE 12.0 NEW SUNSET GLOW NEW RARE AUTHENTIC LIGHTWEIGHT,NEW Women's Adidas Ultra Boost ST Running Shoes Primeknit Navy Breeze BA7832 NEW,New Balance Women's 790v6 Running Shoe Size 8 B (US) Spectral/Gem,Nike Women's Air Zoom Pegasus 35 Phantom Gunsmoke Summit White Run 942855-004,Skechers Women's YOU Serene Slip-on Sneaker Dark Taupe Walking Shoes,Nike Dunk Sky Hi Sail Blue Hidden Wedge Runner Shoe Sz 7.5 EXCELLENTASICS Running Shoes LADY GEL-KAYANO24 TJG758 Dark Blue US8.5(25.5cm),