NWT! VANS Sk8-Hi Top Checkered Sidewall Mens Black/White Shoes Womens 8 Mens Sidewall 6.5 58e949

NWT! VANS Sk8-Hi Top Checkered Sidewall Mens Black/White Shoes Womens 8 Mens Sidewall 6.5 58e949

Item specifics

Condition:
New without box: A brand-new, unused, and unworn item (including handmade items) that is not in original packaging or ... Read moreabout the condition
Product Line: VANS Sk8-Hi
Heel Height: Low (3/4 in. to 1 1/2 in.) Brand: VANS
Width: Medium (B, M) US Shoe Size (Women's): US 8
Color: Black Shoe Height: High Top
This is a cropped closeup of a senior couple enjoying a game of chess

NWT! VANS Sk8-Hi Top Checkered Sidewall Mens Black/White Shoes Womens 8 Mens Sidewall 6.5 58e949

Women’s cognitive functioning past middle age may be affected by the degree of gender equality in the country they live in, according to new findings from Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

“This research is a first attempt to shed light on important, but understudied, adverse consequences of gender inequality on women’s health in later life,” explains researcher Eric Bonsang of University Paris-Dauphine and Columbia University, lead author on the study. “It shows that women living in gender-equal countries have better cognitive test scores later in life than women living in gender-unequal societies. Moreover, in countries that became more gender-equal over time, women’s cognitive performance improved relative to men’s.”

Bonsang and colleagues Vegard Skirbekk (Norwegian Institute of Public Health and Columbia University) and Ursula Staudinger (Columbia University) had noticed that the differences in men’s and women’s scores on cognitive tests varied widely across countries. In countries in Northern Europe, for example, women tend to outperform men on memory tests, while the opposite seems to be true in several Southern European countries.

“This observation triggered our curiosity to try to understand what could cause such variations across countries,” says Bonsang.

While economic and socioeconomic factors likely play an important role, Bonsang, Skirbekk, and Staudinger wondered whether sociocultural factors such as attitudes about gender roles might also contribute to the variation in gender differences in cognitive performance around the globe. They hypothesized that women who live in a society with more traditional attitudes about gender roles would likely have less access to opportunities for education and employment and would, therefore, show lower cognitive performance later in life compared with men of the same age.

The researchers analyzed cognitive performance data for participants between the ages of 50 and 93, drawn from multiple nationally representative surveys including the US Health and Retirement Study; the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe; the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing; and the World Health Organization Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health. Together, the surveys provided data for a total of 27 countries.

All of the surveys include an episodic memory task to measure cognitive performance. Participants heard a list of 10 words and were asked to recall as many as they could immediately; in some of the surveys, participants again recalled as many words as they could after a delay. Additionally, some of the surveys included a task intended to assess executive function in which participants named as many animals as they could within 1 minute.

To gauge gender-role attitudes, the researchers focused on participants’ self-reported agreement with the statement, “When jobs are scarce, men should have more right to a job than women.”

Overall, the data showed considerable variability in gender differences in cognitive performance across countries. In some countries, women outperformed men—the female advantage in cognitive performance was highest in Sweden. In other countries, however, men outperformed women—the male advantage was highest in Ghana.

As the researchers hypothesized, increasingly traditional gender-role attitudes were linked with decreasing cognitive performance among women across countries. In other words, women in countries with less traditional attitudes were likely to have better cognitive performance later in life relative to women in more traditional countries.

Bonsang and colleagues noted that changes in gender-role attitudes within a country over time were associated with changes in women’s cognitive performance relative to men.

Although the data are correlational in nature, several more detailed analyses point toward a causal relationship. These analyses suggest that gender-role attitudes may play a notable role in important outcomes for women across different countries, the researchers argue.

“These findings reinforce the need for policies aiming at reducing gender inequalities as we show that consequences go beyond the labor market and income inequalities,” says Bonsang. “It also shows how important it is to consider seemingly intangible influences, such as cultural attitudes and values, when trying to understand cognitive aging.”

“In future work, we plan to disentangle the effect of gender-role attitudes on gender difference in cognition—via the impacts of those attitudes on institutions, politics and labor market characteristics—from the impact of beliefs of women associated with gender-role attitudes,” Bonsang says.

We use data from Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) Release 2.6.0, English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) Release 19, the RAND Corporation Health and Retirement Study (HRS) Version N, and Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health (SAGE) Wave 1. The SHARE project has been primarily funded by the European Commission (see http://www.share-project.org for the full list of funders). The ELSA project has been funded by a consortium of U.K. government departments and the U.S. National Institute on Aging (see http://www.elsa-project.ac.uk/funders for the full list of funders). The HRS project is funded by the Social Security Administration and the U.S. National Institute on Aging


Published July 31, 2017
Air Jordan 7 VII Retro GS Raptors Bred Purple Youth 2012,Womens NIKE ZOOM SPAN 2 Dark Grey Running Trainers 909007 003HOKA ONE ONE ARAHI VIRTUAL PINK/ FUCHSIA RUNNING SHOES, US 7/ /3 ~NWT,PUMA Womens Basket Remaster Wns Fashion Sneaker- Pick SZ/Color.,DC Women's Lodge Boot Sneaker,Black/Turquoise,7 M US,Skechers 788 Street Womens Double up-Glitzy Gal Sneaker- Choose SZ/Color.,New Balance 775v3 Running Course Shoes Black Women Sz 6.5 B 1128,SKECHERS USA Inc Variation Skechers Womens Burst TR Close Knit,LANVIN Womens Quilted LEATHER+WATERSNAKE Cap-Toe High Top Sneaker Cream 11/41,Womens NIKE Air ZOOM Odyssey Running Trainers 749339 501,NIKE AIR MAX 2014 Running Sneaker 6Y Women Size 7.5 Purple Black Orange,Nike Air Max 1 Ultra Moire Womens Size 7.5. Pink MSRP $130,Nike Flex Supreme TR 5 Women's Training Running Shoes 852467-400 size 9.5,Sperry Women's Seacoast Core Fashion Sneaker - Choose SZ/Color,Asics Gel Quantum 360 Shift Women's Sz 5 Flyknit Running Shoe New With Box,New Balance WRT300IN D Wide Cotton Denim Navy Women Court Classics WRT300IND,New Balance WW1065GR Women's Comfortable Walking Shoe Gray Mesh Upper,Tory Burch Laney Neoprene Slip-On Sneaker Size 9.5,NEW BALANCE 574 - WOMEN'S RED size 5.5 crimson/atlantic,Skechers Skecher Street Womens Zinger-Flicker Sneaker- Select SZ/Color.,PUMA Women's Match LO Basic Sports WN's Tennis Shoe - Choose SZ/ColorDIADORA HERITAGE Mens Womens Sneakers Shoes TRIDENT EVO Red Suede Trainers,adidas Originals Tubular Shadow J Grey White Kid Junior Running Shoes AC8434,NOT TOO COY PLAYFUL FUCHSIA WOMAN SIZE 8.5 STYLISH NEW,adidas Wmns Athletics 24/7 Trainer Women New Green White Training Shoes AH2161,Womens NIKE AIR MAX 2016 PRINT Blue Running Trainers 818101 401,Vintage Nike Womens Dunk Low 2006 Birch Atomic Red Light Chocolate US 8.5 7 SB,John Galliano 2954 Shoes Leather White 180007Salming Miles Women's Running Shoe - SS16 - 10.5 - Pink,Reebok AR2817 Womens Zpump Fusion 2.5 Walking Shoe- Choose SZ/Color.,